Key points
- The Flynn Effect, the increase in intelligence over generations, appears to be stagnating or even reversing in parts of Europe and the U.S.
- Researchers cross-sectionally examined the Flynn Effect in a large dataset of almost 400,000 Americans.
- Cognitive ability scores decreased in several domains related to reasoning, while increasing for visuospatial skills.
- Potential reasons were offered for these results, however, there are alternative explanations, such as the test-taking environment.
Since the early 1900s, intelligence has been increasing by approximately 3 to 5 points per decade; this is likely due to many factors including better nutrition, increased access to education, and familiarity with test-taking. This increase in intelligence over generations is well-documented and has been termed the "Flynn Effect.”
However, recent research in parts of Europe and the U.S. has started to see a plateau in intelligence scores, and even a reversal of the effect. For example, a recent study in the U.S. found lower vocabulary scores in newer cohorts. Shockingly, U.S. college graduates in the 2010s had lower vocabulary scores than their counterparts in the 1970s. Thus, to comprehensively test for the Flynn effect in recent years, Dworak et al. (2023) conducted a pre-registered study of almost 400,000 American adults between 2006-2018.
The researchers gathered cross-sectional data through the Synthetic Aperture Personality Assessment Project, which was advertised online as a personality test. They measured several domains of cognitive ability, including matrix reasoning (non-verbal reasoning), letter and number series (computational-mathematical reasoning), verbal reasoning, and 3D rotation (visuospatial skills).
Results indicated a reversal of the Flynn Effect for composite scores of cognitive ability, matrix reasoning, and letter and number series when participants were grouped by age, education, and gender. In other words, there was a downward trend in these domains of cognitive ability over generations. Yet performance on 3D rotations increased over time. The greatest differences were within groups of 18- to 22-year-olds as well as those without a college degree. To explain their results, the researchers offered five main explanations:
1. The ceiling effect: Intelligence cannot grow forever and must eventually reach a plateau.
The researchers note that prior gains in intelligence were likely due to increases in nutrition, access to education, and familiarity with tests. At a certain point, these gains should plateau, as should performance on intelligence tests. In addition, while prior researchers have suggested decreases in health in recent years, poor health is unlikely to explain the patterns in this study given that 3D rotation abilities actually increased.
2. Media exposure may be responsible for lower scores.
Given the different findings by domain, media exposure might be partially responsible. However, this explanation is currently speculative, as there is little experimental research on the effects of media exposure on intelligence. In addition, no potential mechanisms for the effects of media exposure on cognitive abilities were offered by the researchers.
3. Technology may be protecting or enhancing certain aspects of cognitive performance.
Fortunately, there is a bright side to technology. The researchers propose that certain aspects of technology might explain the increase in 3D rotation abilities, which reflect visuospatial skills. Specifically, video games have been linked to greater visuospatial skills.
4. The quality of education may have decreased.
The researchers propose that education has either decreased in quality or changed in content, thus resulting in poorer non-verbal reasoning and mathematical-computational reasoning, as well as stagnation in verbal reasoning.
5. The importance of different skills may have changed.
Finally, the authors speculate that society now places more value on visuospatial abilities and less value on the various forms of reasoning skills.
Importantly, the researchers caution that cognitive ability scores may not actually be reflective of intelligence. In addition, there are several key limitations of the study. Participants self-selected into the study by finding it online, which could lead to differences in cohorts. Specifically, the authors mention that the personality test is now more popular and may thus include more “normal” people. Furthermore, since the study was advertised as a personality test, people might not have been motivated to do well on cognitive tasks.
An Alternative Explanation: The Role of Smartphones
Given the time frame of the study from 2006-2018 and the researchers' method of data collection, I propose an alternative explanation for their results. Within that period, smartphone ownership in the U.S. rose from an estimated 3 percent to 81 percent, according to one source.
The researchers do not appear to have accounted for whether participants took the test on their phones or on a computer. But given the huge increase in smartphone ownership over time, newer cohorts were much more likely to take the test on their phone, especially since they were led to believe it was a personality test. Therefore, they might have been unable to concentrate on complex questions surrounding verbal, mathematical, and non-verbal reasoning.
Meanwhile, 3D rotation tasks might not be as difficult to complete on a phone, and performance may be less affected by distractions. Furthermore, smartphones have been found to deplete our cognitive resources, so even the presence of a smartphone could be problematic when testing cognitive abilities.
These explanations seem especially plausible given that 18- to 22-year-olds experienced the greatest changes in cognitive ability scores. Compared to older adults, this group seems the most likely to own a smartphone and to have taken the test on their phones in recent years. Hopefully, subsequent iterations of this study will shed light on the potential role of smartphones in the results.
Facebook/LinkedIn image: fizkes/Shutterstock
References
Dworak, E. M., Revelle, W., & Condon, D. M. (2023). Looking for Flynn effects in a recent online US adult sample: Examining shifts within the SAPA Project. Intelligence, 98. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.intell.2023.101734